|
|||
|
33:
Learner-Control and Instructional Technologies
|
33.4 Rationale For Learner Control In CBIThere seems to be several philosophical, practical, and theoretical reasons
for allowing learners some control over events occurring during CBI lessons.
In fact, the use of traditional, rather rigidly controlled computer-assisted
instruction may actually run counter to the educational philosophies promoted
by many teachers in the arts and humanities, and which encourage student
exploration and expression. D. W. Hansen (1982/1983) argues that allowing
students more user control will increase the chance these teachers would
want to include computer-based activities in their classes. Additionally, to many educators and instructional designers, the expression
"learner-controlled instruction" suggests a class of instructional
events or tactics intended to increase learner involvement, mental investment
("mind-fulness," as Salomon, 1983, phrased it), and achievement.
The approach is to emphasize the learner's freedom to choose those learning
activities that suit his or her own individual preferences and needs. On a more pragmatic level, Steinberg (1984) says that, if learning is
found to be equivalent in both learner- and com-puter-controlled settings,
design costs should shrink, time spent learning should be reduced, and
attitudes and motiva-tion should become more positive if a learner-controlled
framework for instruction is adopted. Instructional design theorists, too, have found ample reasons for including
provisions for learner control in computer-based designs. In his design
theory, Merrill (1983) prescribes learner control of content (encompassing
curriculum, lesson, and module selection) and of strategy (spanning various
forms of presentation). Bunderson (1974), Faust (1974), and Fine (1972)
each present an assortment of learner-controlled activities derived from
Merrill's early design theory (Merrill, 1973; Reigeluth, 1979). The TICCIT
(Time-Shared Interactive, Computer Controlled Information Television)
system, derived from Merrill's early theory, provides the learner with
many types of options, some of which are dependent on the current course
(such as reviews, menus, quizzes, faster/slower type of feedback, level
of question difficulty, and topic surveys), while others are constant
across any course delivered by the system (such as backward or forward
movement, access to a calculator, access to a glossary, and opportunity
to leave an on-fine comment; there is even a feature that gives the student
the option to "CUSS" at the computer when things go wrong!). Reigeluth and Stein (1983) in their instructional design theory also
hypothesize that "... instruction generally increases in effectiveness,
efficiency, and appeal to the extent that it permits informed learner
control by motivated learners" (p. 362). Federico (1980), too, suggests
that learner control might be a useful alternative to the classic aptitude-by-treatment
interaction approach (see 22.3.3), in that "learners can become system
independent by enabling them to manipulate and accommodate treatments
to their own momentary cognitive requirements" (p. 17). A rationale from a different perspective comes from a survey of adult
learning preferences. Penland (1979) found that the top four reasons why
adults prefer learning on their own were expressed as desires to "set
my own learning pace," "use my own style of learning,"
"keep the learning strategy flexible," and "put my own
structure on the learning project." Discussing the differences between
adults and children, Hannafin (1984) argues that under CBI condi-tions,
older students should realize the benefits from learner control more than
younger students because they have acquired more (and presumably better)
learning strategies. Some of the research from the psychology of basic learning processes also implies possible advantages of learner control. For example, one might expect a learner-controlled instructional treatment to induce more elaborate mental processing by students as a result of their having to ponder the choices they face. Salomon (1983, 1985) refers to the degree of such mental activity as "invested mental effort." The more such effort expended, he implies, the more mental elaborations the student performs, result-ing in deeper, more meaningful learning. In contrast, one might not expect as much cognitive elaboration from students proceeding through a more "passive" instructional treatment. In plain language, learners given control over their instruction might be more likely to think about what they are doing as a result of having to make choices along the way. Hartley (1985), too, argues for the need for more atten-tion to basic
psychological processes when studying the impact of computers on learning,
and he supports the use of learner control of instruction as a means for
students to develop their own cognitive structures. That is, consistent
with a constructivist view of knowledge development (see 7.3), he proposes
that the learning of complex knowledge structures is facilitated when
the learner himself or herself can participate in the construction of
those mental structures. This constructivist approach is also promoted
by Salomon and Gardner (1986) who suggest that "... individuals
mold their own experiences by the traits and goals they bring to the encounter,
the way they apprehend the technol-ogy and the situation, and the particular
volitional choices they make. In so doing, learners, particularly when
given interactive opportunities with computers, are likely to affect the
way these opportunities are going to affect them" (p. 16, emphasis
added). Indeed, the emerging constructivist paradigm of learning as an inventive
learning process rather than an acquisitive one almost requires some type
of attention to the degree and types of control the learners will exert
during the learning process. Lebow (1993), for example, suggests that,
to constructivists, the strategic availability of learner con-trol options
can provide structural support for the values of personal autonomy, personal
relevance, active engagement, and reflectivity, all important characteristics
undergirding the constructivist philosophy of education. (See 7.4.5 for
further discussion of constructivism and learner control.) |
AECT 877.677.AECT
(toll-free) |